Combat troops must minimize the humanness of their enemies in order to kill them. They can’t be effective fighters if they’re distracted by feelings of empathy for opponents. But indifference to the enemy, rather than loathing, may help prevent war crimes and provide troops with a better path back to healthy civilian lives, researchers at ӰƵ propose.
Their hypothesis is based on new work showing how the brain operates when people objectify—that is, think of others as mere objects—or dehumanize, which entails seeing others as disgusting animals.
These two ways of suspending humanity are common. Think of being treated like a number by an insurance company or enduring a boss who deems subordinates incompetent baboons.
“Whether a person objectifies another or views another as a subhuman animal, he suspends his moral concern for that other person,” said Anthony Jack, assistant professor of cognitive science at ӰƵ and leader of the recently published neuroimaging study.
But how the brain is activated in each case is far different—the key to their premise.
To think of another as an object, people deactivate the empathetic network in their brain, and sometimes also activate the analytical network, depending on the complexity of their thought. This seesawing between the two networks is a natural function of the healthy brain. Jack’s earlier research shows the adult brain naturally cycles between the two networks at rest and chooses the appropriate network depending on the task at hand.
To dehumanize another as so animal-like as to evoke disgust causes both networks to become active. But rather than leading to a good mix of empathy and analytics, this kind of thinking is used in antisocial, manipulative behavior and is most closely associated with mental illnesses, from depression to schizophrenia.
But it’s easy to do.
“It’s built in from infancy, and ranges in intensity from a mild feeling of revulsion when we see people eating something we don’t like…” Jack said.
“…Up to utter contempt and the conclusion that it’s OK to kill them,” said Shannon French, the Inamori Professor of Ethics and associate professor of philosophy at ӰƵ and a specialist in military ethics.
Jack and two former ӰƵ undergraduate students, Abigail J. Dawson, now a graduate student at the University of Otago in Dunedin, New Zealand, and Megan E. Norr, now at Georgetown University and recently accepted to a clinical psychology doctorate program at University of California Berkeley, describe the brain’s workings in this week’s online issue of NeuroImage. Jack and French propose how the findings could be applied to the military in a preprint of a paper due to appear in the book: Responsibilities to Protect: Different Perspectives, edited by David Whetham, King’s College London. The papers can be found at .
Dehumanizing has preceded atrocities throughout history, from the Nazis comparing Jews to rats before systematically murdering them to Tamerlan Tsarnaev saying he didn’t understand Americans—that they can’t control themselves—before planting a bomb at the Boston Marathon this spring, the researchers said.
“There’s a kernel of hope in this,” French said, “because it suggests you first have to develop a certain mindset before you can get past the moral reservations we naturally have about killing another human. Killing is harder than some might think.”
Researchers find way of thinking may enable battle but prevent war crimes
FEATURED |
June 12, 2013
STORY BY: EDITORIAL STAFF
STORY BY: EDITORIAL STAFF